1 There is no limit on the variation that is possible among human languages. A O2 O1 True R1 No, variation is constrained: see p.000. O2 False R2 2 A sample of 200 languages represents a better basis for doing typological linguistics than a sample of 50 languages. A O3 O1 True R1 Not always: see p.000. O2 False R2 It can: read p.000 again. O3 Possibly, but not necessarily R3 3 Which of the following is not an implicational universal? (Note that you are not asked whether or not they are valid, just whether they are implicational universals.) A O3 O1 If a language has voiceless nasals it has voiced nasals R1 This is an implicational universal: see p.000. O2 All languages with a trial number distinction in nouns also have a dual distinction R2 This is an implicational universal: see p.000. O3 All languages have consonants and vowels R3 O4 All languages with affricates have fricatives R4 This is an implicational universal: see p.000. 4 Which of the following is a non-absolute universal? (Note that you are not asked whether or not they are valid universals.) A O2 O1 All languages have vowels and consonants R1 This is an absolute universal: see p.000. O2 If a language has affricates it usually has fricatives R2 O3 If a language has clicks, it has nasals R3 This is an absolute universal: see p.000. O4 All langauges have at least one phonemic stop R4 This is an absolute universal: see p.000. 5 Which of the following implicational universals is better formulated as a non-implicational universal: A O2 O1 If a language has verbs, it has adpositions R1 This cannot be formulated as a non-implicational universal (see box on p.000) O2 If a language has more than two consonants, it has more than one vowel R2 O3 If a language has clicks, it has lexical tone R3 This cannot be formulated as a non-implicational universal (see box on p.000) O4 If a language has a large number of consonants it usually also has a large number of vowels R4 This cannot be formulated as a non-implicational universal (see box on p.000) O5 If a language has lexical tone, it also has stress R5 This cannot be formulated as a non-implicational universal (see box on p.000) 6 Based on your knowledge of linguistics, which of the following is least likely to be an absolute universal? A O4 O1 Languages have vowels R1 This is a likely absolute universal: see p.000. O2 Languages have stop phones R2 This is a likely absolute universal: see p.000. O3 If a language has a dual category for pronouns it will also distinguish singular from plural in pronouns R3 This is a likely absolute universal: see p.000. O4 Languages have a voicing distinction for stops R4 7 Based on the data shown in the following figure, which of the generalisations below is not valid? (Each balloon represents a different language.)
[img:word_order.jpg] A O4 O1 The verb tends to occur before the object R1 No, this is valid: there is just one exception. O2 If a language has prepositions it has VSO word order R2 No, this generalization holds universally in the data. O3 If a language has SVO word order it has postpositions R3 No, this is an absolute implicational universal for the data set. O4 If a language has postpositions it has SVO word order R4 O5 If a language has VSO word order it has prepositions R5 No, this is an absolute implicational universal for the data set. 8 Based on the data in the figure in Question 7 (repeated below), is it the case that languages have initial subjects if and only if they have postpositions?
[img:word_order.jpg] A O1 O1 Yes R1 O2 No R2 No, it is the case. Examine the figure again. 9 Based on the data shown in the following figure, which of the generalisations below is valid?
[img:word_order2.jpg] A O4 O1 The verb tends to occur after the subject R1 No, this is valid for just 3 of the 8 languages. O2 If a language has prepositions it has VSO word order R2 No, there is one exception. O3 Languages tend to have both prepositions and initial subjects R3 Not so. There is a clear tendency to have prepositions, but not along with initial subjects. O4 If a language has postpositions the subject precedes the verb R4 O5 If a language has SVO word order it has prepositions R5 No, this is true for just one of the two languages with SVO word order. 10 Based on the data in the above figure (repeated below), is it true that languages have prepositions if and only if the verb precedes the object?
[img:word_order2.jpg] A O2 O1 Yes R1 No. Although it is true that if a language has prepositions the verb precedes the object, there is a language in which the verb precedes the object but has no prepositions. O2 No R2 11 Which type of language is characterised by morphologically complex words in which it is not easy to separate morphemes from one another? A O3 O1 isolating R1 See p.000. O2 agglutinating R2 See p.000. O3 fusional R3 O4 polysynthetic R4 See p.000. 12 Given that təwalamnagʔak means 'I sharpened the knife' in Chukchee, what morphological type would you expect this language to be? A O4 O1 isolating R1 Check the definition of isolating on p.000. O2 agglutinating R2 Check the definition of agglutinating on p.000. O3 fusional R3 Check the definition of fusional on p.000. O4 polysynthetic R4 13 If three number distinctions are marked on nouns in a language by inflections, which category will normally be the unmarked one? A O1 O1 singular R1 O2 dual R2 See p.000. O3 plural R3 See p.000. 14 In a language that distinguishes number inflectionally on nouns, if an irregular noun does not make the distinction, which regular form is the irregular noun most likely to resemble? A O1 O1 singular R1 O2 plural R2 See p.000, and note 2 (p.000). 15 Which of the following is not normally a feature of the unmarked category in an opposition? A O3 O1 Smaller in phonological size than marked category R1 See p.000. O2 More frequent in use than marked category R2 See p.000. O3 Admits fewer distinctions in cross-cutting dimensions R3 O4 Tends to occur where the oppositon is neutralised R4 See p.000. 16 Is it true that the unmarked category in an opposition tends to be found most frequently across languages? A O1 O1 Yes R1 O2 No R2 See p.000. 17 Supposing that the following hierarchy accounts for the use of alienable and inalienable possessive constructions cross-linguistically, which end would you expect inalienable possession to extend out from?
body part > inherent attribute > clothing > kin > personally manufactured artefact > pet animal > other possessee
A O1 O1 Leftmost R1 O2 Rightmost R2 See p.000. 18 If in a given language we find that kin are treated as alienable possessions, what does the hierarchy of Question 17 (repeated below) indicate about clothing?
body part > inherent attribute > clothing > kin > personally manufactured artefact > pet animal > other possessee
A O3 O1 It is also treated as an alienable possession R1 Check your understanding of these hierarchies, and your answer to Question 17. O2 It is treated as an inalienable possession R2 Check your understanding of these hierarchies, and your answer to Question 17. O3 No conclusion can be drawn R3 19 If in a given language we find that kin are treated as inalienable possessions, what does the hierarchy of Question 17 (repeated below) indicate about clothing?
body part > inherent attribute > clothing > kin > personally manufactured artefact > pet animal > other possessee
A O1 O1 It is also treated as an inalienable possession R1 O2 It is treated as an alienable possession R2 Check your understanding of these hierarchies, and your answer to Question 17. O3 No conclusion can be drawn R3 Check your understanding of these hierarchies, and your answer to Question 17. 20 Supposing that body part nouns in a given language were treated as inalienable (if attached to people or aninmals) or alienable (if detached or amputated), and all other nouns as alienable, what modification would you make to the hierarchy of Question 17?
body part > inherent attribute > clothing > kin > personally manufactured artefact > pet animal > other possessee
A O1 O1 Replace body part in the hierarchy by attached body part > detached body part R1 O2 Replace body part in the hierarchy by detached body part > attached body part R2 No, this modification will not account for the facts. O3 Nothing: the hierarchy must be rejected as invalid R3 No, a simple modification to the hierarchy resolves the difficulty. 21 Which case is normally unmarked in ergative-absolutive case systems? A O2 O1 ergative R1 See p.000. O2 absolutive R2 O3 neither R3 See p.000. 22 Is it true that in the majority of fixed word order languages S precedes O. A O1 O1 Yes R1 O2 No R2 It is true. See p.000. 23 Which of the following case-marking systems is inconsistent with Silverstein's hierarchy? A O5 O1 Nominative-accusative system for pronouns; ergative-absolutive for all nouns R1 No, this is consistent with Silverstein's hierarchy (Figure 15.3, p.000). O2 Nominative-accusative for pronouns, proper nouns, kin terms, human nouns and animate nouns; ergative-absolutive for inanimate nouns R2 No, this is consistent with Silverstein's hierarchy (Figure 15.3, p.000). O3 Nominative-accusative for first and second person pronouns; ergative-absolutive for third person pronouns and all nouns R3 No, this is consistent with Silverstein's hierarchy (Figure 15.3, p.000). O4 Nominative-accusative for pronouns, proper nouns, kin terms, and human nouns; ergative-absolutive for inanimate nouns; no marking for animate nouns R4 No, this is consistent with Silverstein's hierarchy (Figure 15.3, p.000) O5 Nominative-accusative system for pronouns; ergative-absolutive for all nouns except inanimate nouns, which get no case-marking R5